World Library  
Flag as Inappropriate
Email this Article

Mentalism (psychology)

Article Id: WHEBN0003119260
Reproduction Date:

Title: Mentalism (psychology)  
Author: World Heritage Encyclopedia
Language: English
Subject: Philosophy of psychology, Knowledge representation and reasoning, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Cognitive psychology, Social cognition
Collection:
Publisher: World Heritage Encyclopedia
Publication
Date:
 

Mentalism (psychology)

In psychology, mentalism is an umbrella term that refers to those branches of study that concentrate on mental perception and thought processes, in other words, cognition, like cognitive psychology. This is in opposition to disciplines, most notably behaviorism, that believe that study of psychology should focus on the structure of causal relationships to conditioned responses, that is to say behaviors, and seek to support this hypothesis through scientific methods and experimentation. Over the course of the history of psychology, mentalism and behaviorism have clashed, with one or the other representing the dominant paradigm of psychological investigation at different times in history.

Neither mentalism nor behaviorism are mutually exclusive fields; elements of one can be seen in the other, perhaps more so in modern times compared to the advent of psychology over a century ago.[1]

Classical mentalism

Mentalism dates back to the very founding of the field of psychology. "Classical Mentalism", as it is sometimes called, tied together many differing schools of psychological thought from the beginning, and introspective techniques were the norm when it came to research, making psychology an inherently subjective field. Prominent figures ranged from Edward Titchener to William James; despite Titchener being a Structuralist and James being of the Functionalist school of thought, both agreed on one thing: consciousness was indisputably the subject matter of psychology, making them both Mentalists.[1]

Behaviorism takes over

Concurrently thriving alongside mentalism since the inception of psychology was the perspective of behaviorism. However, it was not until 1913, when psychologist John B. Watson published his article "Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It" that behaviorism began to steal the spotlight from classical mentalism. Watson's ideas sparked what some have called a paradigm shift in the world of psychology,[2] leading to the objective and experimental study of human behavior, rather than subjective, introspective study of human consciousness—the study of which was seen as impossible to truly do, and the focus on it to that point had only been a hindrance to the field reaching its full potential. For a time, behaviorism would go on to be the dominant force driving psychological thought, advanced by the work of other luminaries such as Ivan Pavlov, Edward Thorndike, and B.F. Skinner. Mentalism did not simply die; much like how behaviorism had coexisted beside mentalism earlier in history, so too did mentalism continue to exist, just not as the current ruling theory of scientific psychological thought.

The new mentalism

While behaviorism remains a thriving, vibrant field to this day, a scathing review of B.F. Skinner's "Verbal Behavior" by Noam Chomsky in 1959 heralded a shift back to a focus on consciousness in psychology with the beginning of the cognitive revolution.[3] Critical to the successful revival of the mind or consciousness as the primary focus of study in psychological inquiry were advances in the computer sciences and neurosciences, which allowed for actual brain mapping, among other things. This gave mentalism an objectively experimental way to begin to study the mind, effectively nullifying the main criticism that led to its marginalization half a century earlier.

References

  1. ^ a b Neomentalism. Paivio, Allan Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, Vol 29(4), Dec 1975, 263-291. doi: 10.1037/h0082031
  2. ^ The impact and promise of the cognitive revolution. Sperry, Roger W. American Psychologist, Vol 48(8), Aug 1993, 878-885. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.48.8.878
  3. ^
This article was sourced from Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. World Heritage Encyclopedia content is assembled from numerous content providers, Open Access Publishing, and in compliance with The Fair Access to Science and Technology Research Act (FASTR), Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., Public Library of Science, The Encyclopedia of Life, Open Book Publishers (OBP), PubMed, U.S. National Library of Medicine, National Center for Biotechnology Information, U.S. National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health (NIH), U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, and USA.gov, which sources content from all federal, state, local, tribal, and territorial government publication portals (.gov, .mil, .edu). Funding for USA.gov and content contributors is made possible from the U.S. Congress, E-Government Act of 2002.
 
Crowd sourced content that is contributed to World Heritage Encyclopedia is peer reviewed and edited by our editorial staff to ensure quality scholarly research articles.
 
By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. World Heritage Encyclopedia™ is a registered trademark of the World Public Library Association, a non-profit organization.
 


Copyright © World Library Foundation. All rights reserved. eBooks from Project Gutenberg are sponsored by the World Library Foundation,
a 501c(4) Member's Support Non-Profit Organization, and is NOT affiliated with any governmental agency or department.