World Library  
Flag as Inappropriate
Email this Article

Rod monochromacy

Article Id: WHEBN0007318091
Reproduction Date:

Title: Rod monochromacy  
Author: World Heritage Encyclopedia
Language: English
Subject: Color blindness, List of diseases (R)
Collection:
Publisher: World Heritage Encyclopedia
Publication
Date:
 

Rod monochromacy

Monochromacy
Classification and external resources
Monochromacy is a disease state in human vision, but is normal in pinnipeds (such as Neophoca cinerea shown here), cetaceans, Owl Monkeys and some other animals

Monochromacy, also known as "total color blindness",[1] is a complete inability to distinguish colors.[2] This is distinguished from more common forms of color blindness, in which the affected individual can perceive color differences, but cannot make the same distinctions between colors as can an unaffected person.

Organisms with monochromacy are called monochromats. The perceptual effect of any arbitrarily chosen light from the visible spectrum can be matched by any pure spectral light.

Causes

In vertebrates there are typically two kinds of receptors: rods, which primarily distinguish between levels of illumination, and cones, which are responsible for perception of color. There are different types of cones; each perceives only a single color. The normal explanation of monochromacy is that the organism's retina contains only a single kind of light receptor cell, or at least that only one kind is active at any particular level of illumination. Monochromacy is caused by either a defect or the complete absence of the retinal cones.[1]

Some individuals possess diseases or injuries that lead to nyctalopia, or night blindness, where rod cells stop responding properly to light.

Types

There are two known types of monochromacy.[3][4] "Animals with monochromatic vision may be either rod monochromats or cone monochromats. These monochromats contain photoreceptors which have a single spectral sensitivity curve."[5] There is also a third, theoretical type that has never been identified.

  • Rod monochromacy is the condition of having only rods in the retina. A rod monochromat is truly unable to see any color and can see only shades of grey.
  • Cone monochromacy is the condition of having both rods and cones, but only a single kind of cone. A cone monochromat can have good pattern vision at normal daylight levels, but will not be able to distinguish hues. In humans, who have three different types of cones, there are three differing forms of cone monochromacy.[2] There are three types named according to the single functioning cone class:
  1. Blue cone monochromacy, also known as S-cone monochromacy[1]
  2. Green cone monochromacy, also known as M-cone monochromacy[1]
  3. Red cone monochromacy, also known as L-cone monochromacy[1]
  • Cone monochromacy, type II, if its existence were established, would be the case in which the retina contains no rods, and only a single type of cone. Such an animal would be unable to see at all at lower levels of illumination, and of course would be unable to distinguish hues. In practice it is hard to produce an example of such a retina, at least as the normal condition for a species.

Animals that are monochromats

It used to be confidently claimed that most mammals other than primates were monochromats. In the last half-century, however, evidence of at least dichromatic color vision in a number of mammalian orders has accumulated. Two of the orders of sea mammals, the pinnipeds (which includes the seal, sea lion, and walrus) and cetaceans (which includes dolphins and whales) clearly are cone monochromats, since the short-wavelength sensitive cone system is genetically disabled in these animals. The same is true of the owl monkeys, genus Aotus.

Both rod and cone monochromacy occur as very rare forms of color blindness in humans. Rod monochromacy, or maskun, is the more common of the two. The majority of people described as color blind, however, are either dichromats or anomalous trichromats.

Monochromat capability

According to Jay Neitz, a renowned color vision researcher at the University of Washington, each of the three standard color-detecting cones in the retina of trichromatsblue, green and red — can pick up about 100 different gradations of color. The brain can process the combinations of these three values so that the average human can distinguish about one million different hues.[6] Therefore, a monochromat would be able to distinguish about 100 colors.[7]

See also

References

This article was sourced from Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. World Heritage Encyclopedia content is assembled from numerous content providers, Open Access Publishing, and in compliance with The Fair Access to Science and Technology Research Act (FASTR), Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., Public Library of Science, The Encyclopedia of Life, Open Book Publishers (OBP), PubMed, U.S. National Library of Medicine, National Center for Biotechnology Information, U.S. National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health (NIH), U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, and USA.gov, which sources content from all federal, state, local, tribal, and territorial government publication portals (.gov, .mil, .edu). Funding for USA.gov and content contributors is made possible from the U.S. Congress, E-Government Act of 2002.
 
Crowd sourced content that is contributed to World Heritage Encyclopedia is peer reviewed and edited by our editorial staff to ensure quality scholarly research articles.
 
By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. World Heritage Encyclopedia™ is a registered trademark of the World Public Library Association, a non-profit organization.
 


Copyright © World Library Foundation. All rights reserved. eBooks from Project Gutenberg are sponsored by the World Library Foundation,
a 501c(4) Member's Support Non-Profit Organization, and is NOT affiliated with any governmental agency or department.